Difference between revisions of "Feature focusing"
(→Examples) |
|||
(16 intermediate revisions by 3 users not shown) | |||
Line 1: | Line 1: | ||
==Brief statement of principle== | ==Brief statement of principle== | ||
− | Instruction leads to more robust learning when it guides the learner's attention ("focuses") toward relevant [[features]] of the material, as opposed to unfocused instruction or instruction that guides attention toward | + | Instruction leads to more robust learning when it guides the learner's attention ("focuses") toward relevant [[features]] of the material, as opposed to unfocused instruction or instruction that guides attention toward irrelevant [[features]]. |
+ | |||
==Description of principle== | ==Description of principle== | ||
This principle involves encouraging students to focus on the key [[knowledge components]] in the educational material they are studying. Feature focusing instruction may help students to learn [[knowledge components]] with higher [[feature validity]]. More geneally, attention [[focusing]] may also result in students spending more time during a [[learning events|learning event]] on a particular [[knowledge component]] and thus increase its [[strength]]. | This principle involves encouraging students to focus on the key [[knowledge components]] in the educational material they are studying. Feature focusing instruction may help students to learn [[knowledge components]] with higher [[feature validity]]. More geneally, attention [[focusing]] may also result in students spending more time during a [[learning events|learning event]] on a particular [[knowledge component]] and thus increase its [[strength]]. | ||
Line 6: | Line 7: | ||
Instruction that guides the student to key [[knowledge components]] will result in superior [[long-term retention]] than unfocused instruction. | Instruction that guides the student to key [[knowledge components]] will result in superior [[long-term retention]] than unfocused instruction. | ||
===Examples=== | ===Examples=== | ||
− | Examples include the [[Note-Taking: Focusing On Concepts| Note-Taking]] studies in the Refinement and Fluency cluster. | + | Examples include: |
+ | *the [[Note-Taking: Focusing On Concepts| Note-Taking]] studies in the Refinement and Fluency cluster. | ||
+ | *basic skills in [[Chinese pinyin dictation]] | ||
+ | *learning [[French gender cues]] | ||
+ | *[[MacWhinney - Second Language Grammar]] | ||
+ | * [[Learning the role of radicals in reading Chinese]] | ||
+ | * [[Learning a tonal language: Chinese]] | ||
==Experimental support== | ==Experimental support== | ||
===Laboratory experiment support=== | ===Laboratory experiment support=== | ||
− | Two note-taking studies have found that when copy-pasting notes, students perform better on both [[Normal post-test|Normal post-tests]] and [[long-term retention]] tests when they make selections that include only single ideas, rather than multiple ideas. While behavioral interventions have been effective in reducing selection size, they have not produced increased learning outcomes. With regards to note-taking students' ability to identify key [[knowledge components]] may be limited by their understanding of the material, rather than the note-taking interface. | + | Two [[Note-Taking: Focusing On Concepts| note-taking studies]] have found that when copy-pasting notes, students perform better on both [[Normal post-test|Normal post-tests]] and [[long-term retention]] tests when they make selections that include only single ideas, rather than multiple ideas. While behavioral interventions have been effective in reducing selection size, they have not produced increased learning outcomes. With regards to note-taking students' ability to identify key [[knowledge components]] may be limited by their understanding of the material, rather than the note-taking interface. |
===In vivo experiment support=== | ===In vivo experiment support=== | ||
+ | |||
+ | See the following PSLC studies: | ||
+ | * [[FrenchCulture]] | ||
+ | * [[Learning the role of radicals in reading Chinese]] | ||
+ | * [[Chinese pinyin dictation]] | ||
+ | * [[Learning a tonal language: Chinese]] | ||
+ | |||
+ | (The authors of these studies should add a sentence or two summary of their findings in this section as well as update other relevant sections on this page.) | ||
+ | |||
==Theoretical rationale== | ==Theoretical rationale== | ||
− | + | Feature focusing is a [[refinement]] process, where students are identifying the key ideas, and rejecting irrelevant or unimportant ideas. With regards to feature focusing in learning of explicit textual content, focusing on key ideas may be related to previous results where summaries were found to produce superior outcomes to full text (see Reder & Anderson). | |
+ | |||
==Conditions of application== | ==Conditions of application== | ||
+ | |||
==Caveats, limitations, open issues, or dissenting views== | ==Caveats, limitations, open issues, or dissenting views== | ||
==Variations (descendants)== | ==Variations (descendants)== | ||
==Generalizations (ascendants)== | ==Generalizations (ascendants)== | ||
==References== | ==References== | ||
+ | * Bauer, A., Koedinger, K. 2008. Note-Taking, Selecting, and Choice: Designing Interfaces that Encourage Smaller Seelctions. Joint Conference on Digital Libraries (JCDL 2008). To Appear. | ||
+ | * Bauer, A., Koedinger, K. 2007. Selection-Based Note-Taking Applications. Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’07). 981-990. | ||
+ | * Bauer, A., Koedinger, K. 2006. Pasting and Encoding: Note-taking in Online Courses. IEEE ICALT ’06. 789-793. | ||
+ | * Reder, L. M. & Anderson, J. R. (1980a). A comparison of texts and their summaries: memorial consequences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 198, 121-134. | ||
[[Category:Glossary]] | [[Category:Glossary]] | ||
[[Category:Instructional Principle]] | [[Category:Instructional Principle]] | ||
[[Category:Refinement and Fluency]] | [[Category:Refinement and Fluency]] |
Latest revision as of 22:05, 4 December 2009
Contents
Brief statement of principle
Instruction leads to more robust learning when it guides the learner's attention ("focuses") toward relevant features of the material, as opposed to unfocused instruction or instruction that guides attention toward irrelevant features.
Description of principle
This principle involves encouraging students to focus on the key knowledge components in the educational material they are studying. Feature focusing instruction may help students to learn knowledge components with higher feature validity. More geneally, attention focusing may also result in students spending more time during a learning event on a particular knowledge component and thus increase its strength.
Operational definition
Instruction that guides the student to key knowledge components will result in superior long-term retention than unfocused instruction.
Examples
Examples include:
- the Note-Taking studies in the Refinement and Fluency cluster.
- basic skills in Chinese pinyin dictation
- learning French gender cues
- MacWhinney - Second Language Grammar
- Learning the role of radicals in reading Chinese
- Learning a tonal language: Chinese
Experimental support
Laboratory experiment support
Two note-taking studies have found that when copy-pasting notes, students perform better on both Normal post-tests and long-term retention tests when they make selections that include only single ideas, rather than multiple ideas. While behavioral interventions have been effective in reducing selection size, they have not produced increased learning outcomes. With regards to note-taking students' ability to identify key knowledge components may be limited by their understanding of the material, rather than the note-taking interface.
In vivo experiment support
See the following PSLC studies:
- FrenchCulture
- Learning the role of radicals in reading Chinese
- Chinese pinyin dictation
- Learning a tonal language: Chinese
(The authors of these studies should add a sentence or two summary of their findings in this section as well as update other relevant sections on this page.)
Theoretical rationale
Feature focusing is a refinement process, where students are identifying the key ideas, and rejecting irrelevant or unimportant ideas. With regards to feature focusing in learning of explicit textual content, focusing on key ideas may be related to previous results where summaries were found to produce superior outcomes to full text (see Reder & Anderson).
Conditions of application
Caveats, limitations, open issues, or dissenting views
Variations (descendants)
Generalizations (ascendants)
References
- Bauer, A., Koedinger, K. 2008. Note-Taking, Selecting, and Choice: Designing Interfaces that Encourage Smaller Seelctions. Joint Conference on Digital Libraries (JCDL 2008). To Appear.
- Bauer, A., Koedinger, K. 2007. Selection-Based Note-Taking Applications. Proceedings of the ACM Conference on Human Factors in Computing Systems (CHI ’07). 981-990.
- Bauer, A., Koedinger, K. 2006. Pasting and Encoding: Note-taking in Online Courses. IEEE ICALT ’06. 789-793.
- Reder, L. M. & Anderson, J. R. (1980a). A comparison of texts and their summaries: memorial consequences. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 198, 121-134.