Does learning from worked-out examples improve tutored problem solving?

From LearnLab
Revision as of 19:05, 19 March 2008 by Ron-Salden (talk | contribs) (Study 2 (lab study at Freiburg, Geometry Cognitive Tutor))
Jump to: navigation, search

Does learning from worked-out examples improve tutored problem solving?

Alexander Renkl, Rolf Schwonke, Vincent Aleven, & Ron Salden

Abstract

In our research we combine two educational research branches. The first concerns the Cognitive Tutor research branch which deals with offering students the possibility to attain and extend their skills through means of a computerized curriculum of problems. The second branch concerns the research done on worked-out examples and the fading of worked-out steps. Because students’ prior knowledge on novice elements is usually low it is widely believed presenting a worked-out example might give the student more grasp on the novice elements. By presenting more information about the novice elements the student will not have to bridge the gap between his prior knowledge and the novice elements entirely by himself.

However, as the students progresses through a curriculum his knowledge will grow and fully worked-out examples might give him redundant information. Hence, as the curriculum advances the number of worked-out steps in a problem is gradually decreased until all steps are left blank (i.e., problem solving). By continually fading the worked-out steps the student will have more optimal learning instances than either from only fully worked-out problems or only problem solving in the curriculum. Our studies address the main hypothesis which states that an example enriched Cognitive Tutor can create more deep conceptual understanding.

In our recent In Vivo Study (Study 4) we compared a control condition with two experimental conditions which faded worked-out examples. In the control condition the students had to solve problems and enter explanations for each step as they are used to do. In the fixed fading condition the same preset fading of worked-out steps was applied to all students. In the adaptive fading condition the fading of the worked-out steps was based on the individual student's progress as measures on their problem solving and their reasoning. The data of this study have been collected and are currently being processed to the Datashop after which data analysis will commence.

Background and Significance

The background of this research is twofold. (1) The very successful approach of Cognitive Tutors (Anderson, Corbett, Koedinger, & Pelletier, 1995; Koedinger, Anderson, Hadley, & Mark, 1997) is taken up. These computer-based tutors provide individualized support for learning by doing (i.e., solving problems) by selecting appropriate problems to-be-solved, by providing feedback and problem-solving hints, and by on-line assessment of the student’s learning progress. Cognitive Tutors individualize the instruction by selecting problems based on a model of the students’ present knowledge state that is constantly updated, through a Bayesian process called “knowledge tracing” (Corbett & Anderson, 1995). Although problem solving supported by cognitive tutors has been shown to be successful in fostering initial acquisition of cognitive skill, this approach does not seem to be optimal with respect to focusing the learner on the domain principles to be learned. (2) The research tradition on worked-out examples rooted in Cognitive Load Theory (Sweller, van Merriënboer, & Paas, 1998) and, more specifically, the instructional model of example-based learning by Renkl and Atkinson (in press) are taken up in order to foster skill acquisition that is found in deep conceptual udnerstanding. By presenting examples instead of problems to be solved in the beginning of a learning sequence, the learner have more attentential capacity availabel in order to self-explain and thus deepen their understanding of problem solutions.

In order to foster a deep understanding of domain principles and how they are applied in problem solving, we combine the theoretical rationales of Cognitive Tutors and example-based learning.

Especially, we address the following main hypotheses:

  1. Enriching a Cognitive Tutor unit with examples whose worked-out steps are gradually faded leads to better learning.
  2. Individualizing the fading procedure based on the quality of self-explanations that the learners provide further improves learning.
  3. Using free-form self-explanations is more useful in this context as compared to the usual menu-based formats.
  4. Learning can be enhanced further by providing previously self-explained examples – including the learner’s own self-explanations – as support at problem-solving impasses.

This project is in several respects of signficance:

(1) Presently, the positive effects of examples were shown in comparison to unsupported problem solving. We aim to show that example study is also superior to supported problem solving in the very beginning of a learning sequence.

(2) The Cognitive Tutor approach can be enhanced by ideas from research on example-based learning.

(3) The example-based learning approach can be enriched by individualizing instructinal procedures such as fading.

Glossary

Salden Learning-from-Examples Glossary

Learning by worked-out examples

Learning by problem solving

Self-explanation

Fading

Research question

Can the effectiveness and efficiency of Cogntive Tutors be enhanced by including learning from worked-out examples?

Independent variables

The independent variable refers to the following variation:

(a) Cognitive Tutor with problems to be solved

versus

(b) Cognitive Tutors with intially worked-out examples, then partially worked-out examples, and finally problem to be solved.

(b1) The fading occurs according to a fixed procedure which is used for all students.

(b2) The fading occurs adaptively based on the knowledge and reason tracing of the individual student's progress.

(c) Previously performed problems are accessible as hints. Meaning that the regular textual hints are replaced by screenshots of the previously performed problems.

Although self-explanation prompts are a typical "ingredient" of example-based learning, but not of learning by problem solving, such prompts were included in all conditions of our studies. Thereby, the potential effects can be clearly attributed to the presence or absense of example study. The exception to this is Study 6 which includes two Untutored conditions. For more information on this study please scroll down to the Further Information section.

Dependent variables

1) Normal post-test of procedural knowledge: measured by the students' performance on new problems dealing with the content they learned in the Cognitive Tutor.

1) Transfer test assessing conceptual knowledge: measured by a variety of questions on the post test that are different in format, non-isomorphic, from those used in training. These include drawing problems, multiple questions and open questions concerning the specific geometric principles the students were exposed to in the Cognitive Tutor.

3) Acceleration of future learning (in future experiments): we plan to use a related unit to the angles and/or circles units which follows each of these units in the curriculum.

4) Learning time: measured in total time to complete the unit in the Cognitive Tutor.

5) Efficiency of learning (relating learning time to learning outcomes): based on the mental efficiency measure developed by Paas and van Merriënboer (1993).

Hypotheses & Results

The provision of example in Cognitive Tutors should lead to better conceptual understanding and, thereby, transfer performance. In addition, examples in Cognitive Tutors should reduce learning time.

On the whole, the present results confirm the hypotheses with respect to conceptual knowledge and learning time. The excepted effects on transfer were not found.


Study 1 (lab study at Feiburg, Geometry Cognitive Tutor)

  • Summary
    • Lab Study: 8th grade and 9th grade students from a German high school in Freiburg
    • Domain: translated Circles Unit in the Geometry Cognitive Tutor
    • Start Date: March 1
    • End Date: March 31
    • Number of Students: 50
    • Participant Hours: 1.5
    • Data in Datashop: Yes
  • The students were randomly assigned to one of two conditions:
    • Problem Solving Condition: In this control condition students solved answer steps and entered explanations on all problems

Freiburg Summer 2006 - problem solving versus fixed fading examples study - Problem condition-small.jpg

    • Worked Example Condition: In this experimental condition students were first presented with problems that had worked out (i.e., filled in) answer steps but still had to enter the explanations for these steps. As they progressed through the Unit these worked out answers steps were faded meaning that towards the end of the Unit the students had to fill in answer steps and explanations.

Freiburg Summer 2006 - problem solving versus fixed fading examples study - Example condition-small.jpg

  • Findings
    • No overall effect of experimental condition on students' conceptual and procedural knowledge on the post test: t < 1.
    • However, about the same learning outcomes were achieved in shorter learning times in the example-enriched Cognitive Tutor: t(48) = -3.11, p < .001 (one-tailed), r = .41.
    • Accordingly, the efficiency of learning was superior in this latter learning condition: t(48) = 1.73, p < .05 (one-tailed), r = .24 for conceptual knowlegde, and t(48) = 1.82, p < .05 (one-tailed), r = .25 for the acquisition of transferable knowledge.

Study 2 (lab study at Freiburg, Geometry Cognitive Tutor)

  • Summary
    • Lab Study: 9th grade and 10th grade students from a German high school in Freiburg
    • Domain: translated Circles Unit in the Geometry Cognitive Tutor
    • Start Date: April 1
    • End Date: April 31
    • Number of Students: 30
    • Participant Hours: 1.5
    • Data in Datashop: Yes
  • The students were randomly assigned to one of two conditions:
    • Problem Solving Condition: In this control condition students solved answer steps and entered explanations on all problems. See Study 1 for a screenshot of this condition.
    • Worked Example Condition: In this experimental condition students were first presented with problems that had worked out (i.e., filled in) answer steps but still had to enter the explanations for these steps. As they progressed through the Unit these worked out answers steps were faded meaning that towards the end of the Unit the students had to fill in answer steps and explanations. See Study 1 for a screenshot of this condition.
  • Findings
    • With regard to conceptual understanding part of the transfer post test, an advantage of the example condition over the problem condition was found: t(28) = 1.85, p < .05 (one-tailed), r = 0.33.
    • There were no significant differences on other, procedural, transfer items: t < 1.
    • Similar to Study 1, students in the example condition spent significantly less time than students in the problem solving condition: t(28) = -3.14, p < .001 (one-tailed), r = 0.51.
    • Hence, on a measure of robust learning efficient (i.e., relating performance in terms of the acquisition of conceptual knowledge to the effort in terms of time on task) a large effect was obtained: r = 0.55, t(28) = 3.48, p < .001.

Study 3 (lab study at CMU, Geometry Cognitive Tutor)

  • Summary
    • Lab Study: 8th grade and 9th grade students in rural Pennsylvannia schools
    • Domain: Circles Unit in the Geometry Cognitive Tutor
    • Start Date: October 1
    • End Date: November 30
    • Number of Students: 45
    • Participant Hours: 2
    • Data in Datashop: Yes
  • The students were randomly assigned to one of two conditions:
    • Problem Solving Condition: In this control condition students solved answer steps and entered explanations on all problems
    • Worked Example Condition: In this experimental condition students were first presented with problems that had worked out (i.e., filled in) answer steps but still had to enter the explanations for these steps. As they progressed through the Unit these worked out answers steps were faded meaning that towards the end of the Unit the students had to fill in answer steps and explanations.
  • Findings
    • No overall effect of experimental condition on students' conceptual and procedural knowledge on the post test: t < 1.
    • No difference in time to complete the Circles Unit in the Tutor: t < 1.
    • However, about the same learning outcomes were achieved in shorter learning times in the example-enriched Cognitive


Study 4 (In Vivo study at CMU, Geometry Cognitive Tutor)

  • Summary
    • Lab Study: 10th grade geometry classes in rural Pennsylvannia high school
    • Domain: Angles Unit in the Geometry Cognitive Tutor
    • Start Date: March 1
    • End Date: March 31
    • Number of Students: 51
    • Participant Hours: 5
    • Data in Datashop: Being processed
  • The students were randomly assigned to one of three conditions:
    • Problem Solving Condition: In this control condition students solved answer steps and entered explanations on all problems
    • Fixed Fading of Worked Examples Condition: In this experimental condition students were first presented with problems that had worked out (i.e., filled in) answer steps but still had to enter the explanations for these steps. As they progressed through the Unit these worked out answers steps were faded meaning that towards the end of the Unit the students had to fill in answer steps and explanations.
    • Adaptive Fading of Worked Examples Condition: This experimental condition is similar to the Fixed Fading condition but differs in the fact that the fading of the filled in answer steps is based on the individual student's performance on both the answer and the reason steps.
  • Expected Findings
    • Adaptive Fading of Worked Examples Condition > Fixed Fading of Worked Examples Condition > Problem Solving Condition


Summary of Findings and Explanation

This study belongs to the interactive communication cluster because it investigates a variation of the amount of contribution from the system and from the learner, respectively: Who provides the solution of the initial solution steps?

More specifically, this study is about changes in path choices that occur when a tutoring system includes partially worked examples. The basic idea is that when a tutor relieves a student of most of the work in generating a line by providing part of it, then students are more likely to engage in deep learning to fill in the rest. However, the instruction must be engineered so that students still become autonomous problem solvers—they eventually can do all the work themselves.

In the first German laboratory study the standard Cognitive Tutor was compared with an example-enriched Cognitive Tutor. While no effects on procedural and conceptual knowledge transfer items were found, the students working with the example-enriched Tutor completed the curriculum faster than the students in the standard Cognitive Tutor. Using the learning time to measure the condition efficiency showed that the example-enriched Tutor obtained higher learning efficiency on the transfer test. Since the German students were inexperienced with the Cognitive Tutor more detailed instructions were provided in the follow up study. Consequently the students working on the example-enriched Tutor showed a higher gain on the conceptual knowledge items of the transfer test than the students working with the standard Tutor. Furthermore, similar to the first study the example-enriched Tutor led to significantly shorter learning time than the standard Cognitive Tutor. Lastly, using the learning time to measure efficiency revealed higher learning efficiency for the example-enriched Tutor on the conceptual knowledge items of the transfer test. In terms of the robust learning framework, these results shows that worked out examples lead to same level of foundational skills in less time. Furthermore, the second study shows that fading worked out examples can improve sense-making which consequently leads to better robust learning.

Further Information

Current and upcoming studies up to June 2007

  • Lab Study (Study 5) at Freiburg which features the Tutor used in Study 4 translated into German: currently running
  • Lab Study (Study 6: scheduled on April 18 + 19) at CMU which will compare four conditions:

1. Tutored Problem Solving

2. Tutored Worked Examples

3. Untutored Problem Solving

4. Untutored Worked Examples

  • In Vivo Study (Study 7) at CMU to be run starting April 23 at two rural Pennsylvannia high schools which will compare two conditions:

1. Problem Solving

2. Access to previously solved problems which serve as examples

Future Plans July - December 2007

  • Analyze the data from studies 4 to 7 and write scientific reports on the results
  • Run two experiments, a Lab Study version at Freiburg (Study 8) and an In Vivo Study at CMU (Study 9) with a 2 x 2 design:

factor 1: control-over-examples (fixed fading v. student control)

factor 2: reward-structure (standard knowledge-tracing v. KC-dollars)

Annotated bibliography

Salden R. J. C. M., Aleven, V., Renkl, A., & Wittwer, J. (2006, July). Does Learning from Examples Improve Tutored Problem Solving? In 2006 Proceedings of the 28th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 2602). Vancouver, Canada. Link to paper

Presentation to the PSLC Advisory Board, Fall 2006.

Schwonke, R., Wittwer, J., Aleven, V., Salden, R. J. C. M., Krieg, C., & Renkl, A. (2007). Can tutored problem solving benefit from faded worked-out examples? Paper to be presented at The European Cognitive Science Conference 2007, May 23-27. Delphi, Greece. Link to paper

Salden, R., Aleven, V., & Renkl, A. (2007). Can tutored problem solving be improved by learning from examples? Proceedings of the 29th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (p. 1847), Nashville, USA.

Schwonke, R., Renkel, A., Krieg, C, Wittwer, J., Aleven, V., Salden, R. J. C. M. (2008). The Worked-example Effect: Not an Artefact of Lousy Control Conditions. Computers in Human Behavior.

Salden, R., Aleven, V., Schwonke, R., & Renkl, A. (2008). Worked Are Worked Examples and Tutored Problem Solving Synergistic Forms of Support? Accepted by the 8th International Conference of the Learning Sciences (ICLS).

References

Anderson, J. R., Corbett, A. T., Koedinger, K. R., & Pelletier, R. (1995). Cognitive tutors: Lessons learned. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 4, 167-207.

Corbett, A. T., & Anderson, J. R. (1995). Knowledge tracing: Modeling the acquisition of procedural knowledge. User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction, 4, 253-278.

Koedinger, K. R., Anderson, J. R., Hadley, W. H., & Mark, M. A. (1997). Intelligent tutoring goes to school in the big city. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 8, 30-43.

Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J.J.G. (1993). The efficiency of instructional conditions: An approach to combine mental-effort and performance measures. Human Factors, 35, 737-743.

Renkl, A., & Atkinson, R. K. (in press). Cognitive skill acquisition: Ordering instructional events in example-based learning. F. E. Ritter, J. Nerb, E. Lehtinen, T. O’Shea (Eds.), In order to learn: How ordering effect in machine learning illuminate human learning and vice versa. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

Sweller, J., Merriënboer, J. J. G. van, & Paas, F. G. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10, 251-296.