Difference between revisions of "Fluency Summer Intern Project 2008"

From LearnLab
Jump to: navigation, search
(Background and significance)
(Findings)
Line 72: Line 72:
 
General Linear Model analyses revealed the following significant effects.
 
General Linear Model analyses revealed the following significant effects.
  
Trained correct
+
'''Trained correct'''
Group: F(1, 28) = 11.87, MSE = 1.02, p<.05
+
* Group: F(1, 28) = 11.87, MSE = 1.02, p<.05
Activity: F(2, 56) = 2.74, MSE = .23, p=.07
+
* Activity: F(2, 56) = 2.74, MSE = .23, p=.07
Group*Activity: F(2, 16) = 3.61, MSE = .41, p<.05
+
* Group*Activity: F(2, 16) = 3.61, MSE = .41, p<.05
  
Trained incorrect
+
'''Trained incorrect'''
Group: F(1, 28) = 9.16, MSE = .13, p<.05
+
* Group: F(1, 28) = 9.16, MSE = .13, p<.05
  
Untrained correct
+
'''Untrained correct'''
Group: F(1, 28) = 4.96, MSE = 1.44, p<.05
+
* Group: F(1, 28) = 4.96, MSE = 1.44, p<.05
  
Untrained incorrect
+
'''Untrained incorrect'''
Group: F(1, 28) = .71, MSE = .02, p<.05
+
* Group: F(1, 28) = .71, MSE = .02, p<.05
  
In summary:
+
 
 +
'''In summary:'''
 
* Students in the trained condition used more formulaic sequences than students who were untrained.
 
* Students in the trained condition used more formulaic sequences than students who were untrained.
  
Line 102: Line 103:
 
** Arabic speakers rarely (if ever) used any formulaic sequences.
 
** Arabic speakers rarely (if ever) used any formulaic sequences.
 
** Korean and East Asian speakers used more untrained sequences than trained sequences, in both conditions.
 
** Korean and East Asian speakers used more untrained sequences than trained sequences, in both conditions.
 
  
 
== Explanation ==
 
== Explanation ==

Revision as of 19:46, 1 August 2008

Abstract

The following reports the findings from the summer intern project of Megan Ross (Northwestern University) in June and July 2008. It concerns an analysis of the data gathered in Spring 2008 for Study 2 of the project Fostering fluency in second language learning (De Jong, Halderman, and Perfetti).

Data was analyzed of 30 students of English as a second language, who had received a 50-minute training on formulaic sequences, followed by three sessions of the 4/3/2 procedure, which is designed to promote fluency.

Results showed that …

Background and significance

Formulaic sequences are prefabricated structures of a language which are relatively fixed that help people speak with fluency (Nattinger & DeCarrico, 1992). Speakers use formulaic sequences for several reasons. First of all, they allow for more efficient retrieval of other information (chunking). In addition, both the speaker and listener are able to focus on the “bigger picture” of the conversation, rather than on individual words. Finally, in L2 acquisition, using formulaic sequences helps the student sound more fluent, and provides the student with more time to think about what else they want to say (resource allocation).

Nattinger & DeCarrico distinguish between four types of formulaic sequences (or lexical phrases).

Grammatical level Canonical/Non-canonical Variable/Fixed Continuous/Dis-continuous
Polywords (Ex. For the most part…) word level both fixed continuous
Institutionalized expressions (Ex. Have a nice day) sentence level canonical fixed continuous
Phrasal constraints (Ex. A [long time] ago…) word level both somewhat variable mostly continuous
Sentence builders (Ex. Not only X, but Y) sentence level canonical highly variable often discontinuous

Research questions

Two competing theories about the acquisition and use of formulaic sequences were considered. Two theories of how formulaic sequences are learned (correctly and incorrectly):  Language is proceduralized and automatized in grammatically correct chunks. If the structure of the sequence is incorrect, then the formulaic sequence has not been proceduralized (Towell et al, 1996).  Language is learned holistically for L1, but (older) L2 learners process formulaic sequences at the word level. Consequently, incorrect formulaic sequence use is a result of constructing the sequence from parsed speech (Wray, 2002).

In this study, we investigated whether ESL Students who are trained to use formulaic sequences will use them more often and with greater accuracy, as compared to ESL students who do not receive the training.

Method

The participants in the study were 30 ESL students enrolled in the English Language Institute of the University of Pittsburgh, at levels 3 and 4 (low and high intermediate). At the beginning of the semester they received a 50-minute pretraining in which they learned the form and use of 10 formulaic sequences through listening, fill-in-the-blank exercises, and partner-work.

Subsequently, they performed the 4/3/2 procedure (see Fostering fluency in second language learning) three times during the first half of the Spring 2007 semester. They also performed three Recorded Speaking Activities at the beginning, middle, and end of the semester, which served as pretest, posttest and delayed posttest.


Independent variables

  • a. Pretraining vs. no pretraining of formulaic sequences
In the Formulaic Sequences condition, students receive a short training of a number of formulaic sequences before they start the fluency training (4/3/2 task). In the No Formulaic Sequences condition, students do not receive this pretraining, and only do the 4/3/2 task.
  • b. Low intermediate vs. high intermediate proficiency level
Low intermediate students are enrolled in ELI Speaking courses at level 3, high intermediate at level 4.

Dependent variables

  • Temporal measures of fluency:
    • Articulation rate: number of syllables per second
    • Pauses:
      • mean length of fluent runs between pauses
      • mean length of pauses
      • phonation/time ratio
  • Formulaic sequences:
    • number of correct and incorrect formulaic sequences repeated from training (trained sequences)
    • number of other correct and incorrect formulaic sequences (untrained sequences)
  • Near transfer, immediate and delayed, normal post-test: After completing the last training session, students performed a similar task (spontaneous speech about a given topic), to test whether any gains in fluency during the training task were maintained in a new instance of the same task. This test was given one week and four weeks after the last training session, each time with a different topic. These recordings were made as part of the Recorded Speaking Activities (RSAs) from the project "The self-correction of speech errors (McCormick, O’Neill & Siskin)".


Hypotheses


Findings

General Linear Model analyses revealed the following significant effects.

Trained correct

  • Group: F(1, 28) = 11.87, MSE = 1.02, p<.05
  • Activity: F(2, 56) = 2.74, MSE = .23, p=.07
  • Group*Activity: F(2, 16) = 3.61, MSE = .41, p<.05

Trained incorrect

  • Group: F(1, 28) = 9.16, MSE = .13, p<.05

Untrained correct

  • Group: F(1, 28) = 4.96, MSE = 1.44, p<.05

Untrained incorrect

  • Group: F(1, 28) = .71, MSE = .02, p<.05


In summary:

  • Students in the trained condition used more formulaic sequences than students who were untrained.
  • Students in the trained condition attempted more sequences (incorrect use) than students who were not trained.
  • Untrained sequences were not taught in the training, but when students used the untrained sequences, they were very accurate.
  • Activity 3 may have yielded higher sequence use than Activities 2 and 4 because of the topic.
  • Overall significantly more correct use in both the trained and untrained conditions.
  • Other observations
    • Arabic speakers rarely (if ever) used any formulaic sequences.
    • Korean and East Asian speakers used more untrained sequences than trained sequences, in both conditions.

Explanation


Further information

The data preparation and analysis of this part of the project has been performed by Megan Ross from Northwestern University (summer intern in June-July 2008) and Dr. Laura Halderman from the University of Pittsburgh.