REAP Comparison to Classroom Instruction (Fall 2006)

From LearnLab
Revision as of 19:06, 21 April 2008 by Juffs (talk | contribs)
Jump to: navigation, search

Logistical Information

Contributors Alan Juffs, Lois Wilson, Maxine Eskenazi, Michael Heilman
Study Start Date September 2006
Study End Date April, 2007
Learnlab Courses English Language Institute Reading 4 (ESL LearnLab)
Number of Students ~72
Total Participant Hours (est.) approximately 360
Data in Datashop no


Abstract

This paper focuses on the long-term retention and production of instructed vocabulary in an intensive English program (IEP). The paper draws on the practical framework of Coxhead (2001) and Nation (2005) and the theoretical perspectives of Laufer & Hulstijn (2001). The project collected data from over 150 ESL learners over a period of 1.5 years. The vocabulary instruction occurred in intermediate level reading class (intermediate =TOEFL 450 or iBT TOEFL 45). All learners spent 40 minutes per week for 9 weeks reading texts containing words from the Academic Word List. The words were highlighted and learners could click to access on-line definitions. A subset of the learners' normal in-class vocabulary instruction was tracked for two semesters. Pre-, post and delayed post-test data were collected for the CALL vocabulary learning and the in-class learning. In addition, during this period, all of the students' writing assignments were collected on-line. From this database of written output, each student’s texts were analyzed to determine which words seen during computer training and regular reading class had transferred to their spontaneous output in compositions in their writing class. Results indicate that although the CALL practice led to recognition one semester later, only the words which were practiced during regular reading class vocabulary instruction transferred to their spontaneous writing. This transfer effect is attributed to the output practice and deeper processing that occurred during the regular vocabulary instruction. The data also showed that the production of words seen in the CALL condition alone suffered from errors in word recognition (‘clang’ associations) and morphological form errors (Schmitt & Meara, 1997). We conclude that these data suggest that some negative views on output practice by Folse (2006) and Barcroft (2005) must be modified to accommodate these data.


Introduction

In the fall of 2006 and the spring of 2007, we followed learners using REAP and also tracked the in class vocabulary instruction led by teachers. This study focused on a comparison of REAP with what normally happens in classrooms. In that sense, it is not very strictly controlled study, but it is ecologically valid in the sense that the study reflects what actually happens in classes.


It should also be noted that the REAP treatments in the fall of 2006 and spring 2007 were slightly different. In the fall of 2006, participants were introduced to the personalization by topic. In the fall of 2006, all of the students had their focus words high lighted, but not all students received text that were of interest to them. [1]. Students were all able to select their topic of interest in the spring of 2007, but only half of the students had their focus words highlighted, in contrast to all of the students in the fall of 2006. The spring study is described here. [2].

The Reading 4 curriculum supervisor decided on a list of Academic Word List vocabulary items that had been excluded from the tests that the students took to establish their focus word lists. This list included 58 items that in her view the students should know. These words also appeared in the learners' text books.

In this report, we compare learning gains in REAP in the fall of 2006 with in class learning. In the spring of 2007, we again compare learning gains with the highlighted and non highlighted word treatment groups.

Research Questions


1. What does vocabulary learning look in ‘in vivo’ as opposed to a very tightly controlled study? (action research) 2. How does in class vocabulary instruction differ from the CALL in the reading course in intermediate ESL? 3. How do the learning outcomes differ? 4. If there are differences, what might the source of those differences be? 5. Can deeper processing through writing be ‘skipped’ by using a CALL program?


Participants

Fall term 2006 L1 N MTELP SD

African 2 58.5 19.09 Arabic 18 46.89 7.78 Chinese 4 63.75 12.12 Japanese 1 56.00 . Korean 6 60.83 13.98 Spanish 2 68.00 0 Thai 1 53.70 . TOTAL 34 53.70 12.08

Spring term 2007 L1 N MTELP SD Arabic 10 49.30 11.81 Chinese 7 59.29 11.85 Japanese 2 53.00 8.49 Korean 13 60.34 13.13 Spanish 2 75.00 8.48 Turkish 4 61.75 7.04 TOTAL 34 53.70 12.08

Selected References

Allum, P. (2002). CALL and the classroom: the case for comparative research. ReCALL, 14, 146-166.

Barcroft, J. (2004). Effects of sentence writing in second language lexical acquisition. Second Language Research, 20, 303-334.

Barcroft, J. (2006). Negative Effects of forced output on vocabulary learning. Second Language Research, 22, 487-497.

Folse, K. S. (2006). The effect of type of written exercise on L2 vocabulary retention. TESOL Quarterly, 40, 273-293.

Hulstijn, J., & Laufer, B. (2001). Some empirical evidence for the involvement load hypothesis in vocabulary acquisition. Language Learning, 51, 539-558.

Juffs, A., Friedline, B. F., Eskenazi, M., Wilson, L., & Heilman, M. (in review). Activity theory and computer-assisted learning of English vocabulary. Applied Linguistics.

Stanowicz, K. E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21, 360-407.