Difference between revisions of "Learning the role of radicals in reading Chinese"

From LearnLab
Jump to: navigation, search
(Explanation)
(Abstract)
 
(13 intermediate revisions by the same user not shown)
Line 16: Line 16:
 
== Abstract ==
 
== Abstract ==
  
    Does providing reliable semantic information help second language learners acquire new words? Two experiments investigated whether adult learners of Chinese benefited from explicit instruction of semantic information when learning new characters. We manipulated whether semantic information was a reliable cue to word meaning and whether predictability was taught [[explicit instruction|explicitly]]. We measured learning outcomes with translation and semantic judgment tasks.
+
Does providing reliable semantic information help second language learners acquire new words? Two experiments investigated whether adult learners of Chinese benefited from [[explicit instruction]] of semantic information when learning new characters. We manipulated whether semantic subcomponents were reliable cues to word meanings (reliable vs. unreliable predictors) and whether the semantic information was taught explicitly or not (explicit vs. implicit training conditions). Results showed that explicitly providing semantic cues promoted short-tem retention of target characters and aided somewhat in transferring knowledge to new characters. Reliability of cues had early effects on learning but no effect on [[long-term retention]] or on [[transfer]]. We theorize that learners benefit from explicit instruction of the connection between semantic subcomponents of words and the meanings of individual words, even when semantic cues are unreliable. However, it is still not clear whether early learners apply this knowledge when learning new vocabulary.
  
 
== Glossary ==
 
== Glossary ==
Line 27: Line 27:
  
 
== Background ==
 
== Background ==
A '''background''' and significance section that briefly summarizes prior work on the research question and why it is important to answer it
+
 
  
 
    Previous research has shown that non-native learners of Chinese do not discern the presence of [[cue validity|helpful cues]] in the orthography unless such relationships are taught explicitly (Taft & Chung, 1999). But because semantic cues in Chinese are not always reliable predictors of word meaning (Hanley, 2005; Shu, Chen, Anderson, Wu, & Xuan, 2003), it may actually be more confusing for a beginning learner to be taught these relationships. The aim of this study was to determine how [[reliability]] of cues can affect learning. As in every language, Chinese has rules and exceptions to those rules. The written form of Chinese contains a high percentage of compound characters, which are single, one-syllable words made up of semantic and phonetic radicals. These radicals, or linguistic subcomponents, often provide cues to the character’s meaning and pronunciation. However, a reader cannot rely solely on using this strategy to decode new words in Chinese. Therefore, we wanted to ascertain whether it is helpful to teach the sometimes ambiguous relationship between linguistic subcomponents and whole word definitions.
 
    Previous research has shown that non-native learners of Chinese do not discern the presence of [[cue validity|helpful cues]] in the orthography unless such relationships are taught explicitly (Taft & Chung, 1999). But because semantic cues in Chinese are not always reliable predictors of word meaning (Hanley, 2005; Shu, Chen, Anderson, Wu, & Xuan, 2003), it may actually be more confusing for a beginning learner to be taught these relationships. The aim of this study was to determine how [[reliability]] of cues can affect learning. As in every language, Chinese has rules and exceptions to those rules. The written form of Chinese contains a high percentage of compound characters, which are single, one-syllable words made up of semantic and phonetic radicals. These radicals, or linguistic subcomponents, often provide cues to the character’s meaning and pronunciation. However, a reader cannot rely solely on using this strategy to decode new words in Chinese. Therefore, we wanted to ascertain whether it is helpful to teach the sometimes ambiguous relationship between linguistic subcomponents and whole word definitions.
  
 
== Dependent variables ==
 
== Dependent variables ==
The '''dependent variables''', which are observable and typically measure competence, motivation, interaction, meta-learning, or some other pedagogically desirable outcome
+
 
  
 
[[Normal post-test]] measures:
 
[[Normal post-test]] measures:
- accuracy and response time on a semantic category judgment task with previously learned items (Experiment 1)
 
  
- accuracy of translating previously learned Chinese characters into English (Experiment 2)
+
- accuracy and response time on lexical decision, naming, and semantic category judgment tasks with previously learned items (Experiment 1)
 +
 
 +
- accuracy of definition recognition, accuracy of translating previously learned Chinese characters into English (Experiment 2)
 +
 
 +
[[Transfer]] measures:
  
[[Transfer]] measure:
+
- accuracy on a multiple-choice translation task with new characters (Experiments 1 and 2)
- accuracy on a multiple-choice translation task with new Characters (Experiments 1 and 2)
 
  
 
== Independent variables ==
 
== Independent variables ==
The '''independent variables''', which are typically include instructional environment, activity or method, and perhaps some student characteristics, such as gender or first language
+
 
  
 
    Training condition was either explicit (information was provided about the semantic radical’s meaning in relation to meaning of the character) or implicit (no additional information was provided). Being explicit about the radical is an instance of [[feature focusing]] [[instructional method]]. Each semantic radical was either reliable (its meaning was associated with the meaning of the characters) or unreliable (its meaning was unrelated to the meaning of the character in which it appeared).
 
    Training condition was either explicit (information was provided about the semantic radical’s meaning in relation to meaning of the character) or implicit (no additional information was provided). Being explicit about the radical is an instance of [[feature focusing]] [[instructional method]]. Each semantic radical was either reliable (its meaning was associated with the meaning of the characters) or unreliable (its meaning was unrelated to the meaning of the character in which it appeared).
  
== Hypothesis
+
== Hypothesis ==
The '''hypothesis''', which is a concise statement of the relationship among the variables that answers the research question
+
 
  
 
    We predict an interaction between [[reliability]] and [[explicit instruction|explicitness]], such that learners will perform better on items studied in the explicit condition compared to the implicit condition, and this effect will be greater for characters with reliable semantic radicals than characters with unreliable semantic radicals.
 
    We predict an interaction between [[reliability]] and [[explicit instruction|explicitness]], such that learners will perform better on items studied in the explicit condition compared to the implicit condition, and this effect will be greater for characters with reliable semantic radicals than characters with unreliable semantic radicals.
Line 58: Line 60:
  
 
== Explanation ==
 
== Explanation ==
== An '''explanation''', which is short (a paragraph or two) and typically mentions unobservable, hypothetical attributes of the students (e.g., the students’ knowledge or motivation) and cognitive or social processes that affect them ==
+
 
 
 
   
 
  
  
In Experiment 1, initial learning was better for items with reliable rather than unreliable semantic cues. However, once students were explicitly shown a special characteristic of L2, they could apply it to learning new examples. Our within-subjects design allowed application of explicit strategy to implicit items. In Experiment 2, we used a block design to separate effects of learning before and after explicit instruction. Again, learners did best with reliable items and explicit instruction.
+
In Experiment 1, initial learning was better for items with reliable rather than unreliable semantic cues. However, once students were explicitly shown a special characteristic of L2, they could apply it to learning new examples. Our within-subjects design allowed application of the explicit strategy to implicit items. In Experiment 2, we used a block design to separate effects of learning before and after explicit instruction. Again, learners did best with reliable items and explicit instruction.
 
    
 
    
We theorize that learners benefit from explicit instruction of the connection between semantic subcomponents of words and the meanings of individual words. However, early learners do not seem to apply this knowledge in learning new vocabulary.
+
We theorize that learners benefit from explicit instruction of the connection between semantic subcomponents of words and the meanings of individual words, even when semantic cues are unreliable. However, it is still not clear whether early learners apply this knowledge when learning new vocabulary.
  
 
== Descendents ==
 
== Descendents ==

Latest revision as of 20:26, 26 March 2008


Summary Table

  • Node Title: Semantic Radicals Study
  • Researchers: Susan Dunlap, Ying Liu, Charles Perfetti, Sue-mei Wu
  • PIs: Charles Perfetti, Ying Liu, Min Wang
  • Others who have contributed 160 hours or more:
  • Graduate Students: Susan Dunlap
  • Study Start Date Sep 1, 2005
  • Study End Date Dec 31, 2006
  • LearnLab Site and Courses , CMU Chinese Online
  • Number of Students: 20
  • Total Participant Hours for the study: 60
  • Data in the Data Shop: in progress

Abstract

Does providing reliable semantic information help second language learners acquire new words? Two experiments investigated whether adult learners of Chinese benefited from explicit instruction of semantic information when learning new characters. We manipulated whether semantic subcomponents were reliable cues to word meanings (reliable vs. unreliable predictors) and whether the semantic information was taught explicitly or not (explicit vs. implicit training conditions). Results showed that explicitly providing semantic cues promoted short-tem retention of target characters and aided somewhat in transferring knowledge to new characters. Reliability of cues had early effects on learning but no effect on long-term retention or on transfer. We theorize that learners benefit from explicit instruction of the connection between semantic subcomponents of words and the meanings of individual words, even when semantic cues are unreliable. However, it is still not clear whether early learners apply this knowledge when learning new vocabulary.

Glossary

Semantic radical; Explicit instruction; Implicit instruction; Cue validity

Research Question

Does providing reliable semantic information help second language learners acquire new words?

Background

Previous research has shown that non-native learners of Chinese do not discern the presence of helpful cues in the orthography unless such relationships are taught explicitly (Taft & Chung, 1999). But because semantic cues in Chinese are not always reliable predictors of word meaning (Hanley, 2005; Shu, Chen, Anderson, Wu, & Xuan, 2003), it may actually be more confusing for a beginning learner to be taught these relationships. The aim of this study was to determine how reliability of cues can affect learning. As in every language, Chinese has rules and exceptions to those rules. The written form of Chinese contains a high percentage of compound characters, which are single, one-syllable words made up of semantic and phonetic radicals. These radicals, or linguistic subcomponents, often provide cues to the character’s meaning and pronunciation. However, a reader cannot rely solely on using this strategy to decode new words in Chinese. Therefore, we wanted to ascertain whether it is helpful to teach the sometimes ambiguous relationship between linguistic subcomponents and whole word definitions.

Dependent variables

Normal post-test measures:

- accuracy and response time on lexical decision, naming, and semantic category judgment tasks with previously learned items (Experiment 1)

- accuracy of definition recognition, accuracy of translating previously learned Chinese characters into English (Experiment 2)

Transfer measures:

- accuracy on a multiple-choice translation task with new characters (Experiments 1 and 2)

Independent variables

Training condition was either explicit (information was provided about the semantic radical’s meaning in relation to meaning of the character) or implicit (no additional information was provided). Being explicit about the radical is an instance of feature focusing instructional method. Each semantic radical was either reliable (its meaning was associated with the meaning of the characters) or unreliable (its meaning was unrelated to the meaning of the character in which it appeared).

Hypothesis

We predict an interaction between reliability and explicitness, such that learners will perform better on items studied in the explicit condition compared to the implicit condition, and this effect will be greater for characters with reliable semantic radicals than characters with unreliable semantic radicals.

Findings

In Experiment 1, we found a main effect of reliability, such that participants had higher accuracy on the lexical decision and semantic judgment tasks for items with reliable semantic cues relative to items with unreliable semantic cues. However, we found no effect of training on post-test transfer items.

In Experiment 2, we found a main effect of reliability, such that participants learned the meanings of words with reliable semantic cues better than words with unreliable semantic cues. We also found a main effect of instruction, such that explicit instruction led to more accurate recognition of definitions than implicit instruction. Again, there was no evidence of transfer to new items.

Explanation

In Experiment 1, initial learning was better for items with reliable rather than unreliable semantic cues. However, once students were explicitly shown a special characteristic of L2, they could apply it to learning new examples. Our within-subjects design allowed application of the explicit strategy to implicit items. In Experiment 2, we used a block design to separate effects of learning before and after explicit instruction. Again, learners did best with reliable items and explicit instruction.

We theorize that learners benefit from explicit instruction of the connection between semantic subcomponents of words and the meanings of individual words, even when semantic cues are unreliable. However, it is still not clear whether early learners apply this knowledge when learning new vocabulary.

Descendents

The descendents, which lists links to descendent nodes of this one, if there are any

None yet.

Further information

A further information section that points to documents using hyper links and/or references in APA format. Each indicates briefly the document's relationship to the node (e.g., whether the document is a paper reporting the node in full detail, a proposal describing the motivation and design of the study in more detail, the node for a similar PSLC research study, etc.)

None yet.