Difference between revisions of "Contiguity"

From LearnLab
Jump to: navigation, search
 
(4 intermediate revisions by 2 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
According to the Contiguity Principles, students learn more effectively when text and graphics are tightly integrated, rather than presented separately. The integration can be achieved for example by attaching specific pieces of text to specific parts of an image or diagram by means of call-outs (or text balloons).
+
In multimedia learning, the Contiguity Principle (Mayer, 2001) states that students learn more effectively when text and graphics are tightly integrated, rather than presented separately. Contiguity refers both to spatial and temporal integration.  
  
The concept of contiguity can also be applied to the temporal dimensions.
+
Spatial [[integration]] can be achieved for example by attaching specific pieces of text to specific parts of an image or diagram by means of call-outs or text balloons (spatial contiguity).  ''insert image example here''
 +
 
 +
Temporal contiguity typically is achieved by presenting the to-be-integrated material at the same time, rather than successively. For example, temporal contiguity for diagram & text materials could be achieved by presented text and diagrams simultaneously, rather than successively (e.g., in a multimedia system, showing one screen with text and the next screen with a relevant diagram.  
 +
 
 +
Empirical evidence (and examples) come from the work by Mayer and colleagues.  Typically, these studies involve task where subjects study a declarative presentation of a particular mechanism (e.g., the processes that cause lightning or a brake system), both with a diagram and a textual explanation. The effect of contiguity has often been explained in terms of Cognitive Load Theory.
 +
 
 +
Some researchers use the term "split-attention effect" for the detrimental learning outcomes caused by non-contiguous representations (e.g., Mwangi & Sweller, 1998).
 +
 
 +
Definitions from the [http://edutechwiki.unige.ch/en/Multimedia_animation EdTech Wiki]
 +
* Spatial contiguity principle: Deeper learning when corresponding text and animation are presented near rather than far from each other on the screen
 +
* Temporal contiguity principle: Deeper learning when corresponding narration and animation are presented simultaneously rather than successively
  
Empirical evidence (and examples) come from the work by Mayer and colleagues.  Typically, these studies involve task where subjects study a declarative presentation of a particular mechanism (e.g., the process that causes lightning or a brake system), both with a diagram and a textual explanation.
 
  
 
Mayer, R. (2001). ''Multi-media Learning.'' Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
 
Mayer, R. (2001). ''Multi-media Learning.'' Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
 +
 +
Mwangi, W. & Sweller, J. (1998). Learning to solve compare word problems: The effect of
 +
example format and generating self-explanations. ''Cognition and Instruction, 16,'' 173-199.
  
 
[[Category:Glossary]]
 
[[Category:Glossary]]
 
[[Category:PSLC General]]
 
[[Category:PSLC General]]
 
[[Category:Coordinative Learning]]
 
[[Category:Coordinative Learning]]

Latest revision as of 19:16, 3 March 2008

In multimedia learning, the Contiguity Principle (Mayer, 2001) states that students learn more effectively when text and graphics are tightly integrated, rather than presented separately. Contiguity refers both to spatial and temporal integration.

Spatial integration can be achieved for example by attaching specific pieces of text to specific parts of an image or diagram by means of call-outs or text balloons (spatial contiguity). insert image example here

Temporal contiguity typically is achieved by presenting the to-be-integrated material at the same time, rather than successively. For example, temporal contiguity for diagram & text materials could be achieved by presented text and diagrams simultaneously, rather than successively (e.g., in a multimedia system, showing one screen with text and the next screen with a relevant diagram.

Empirical evidence (and examples) come from the work by Mayer and colleagues. Typically, these studies involve task where subjects study a declarative presentation of a particular mechanism (e.g., the processes that cause lightning or a brake system), both with a diagram and a textual explanation. The effect of contiguity has often been explained in terms of Cognitive Load Theory.

Some researchers use the term "split-attention effect" for the detrimental learning outcomes caused by non-contiguous representations (e.g., Mwangi & Sweller, 1998).

Definitions from the EdTech Wiki

  • Spatial contiguity principle: Deeper learning when corresponding text and animation are presented near rather than far from each other on the screen
  • Temporal contiguity principle: Deeper learning when corresponding narration and animation are presented simultaneously rather than successively


Mayer, R. (2001). Multi-media Learning. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Mwangi, W. & Sweller, J. (1998). Learning to solve compare word problems: The effect of example format and generating self-explanations. Cognition and Instruction, 16, 173-199.